lanky
Well-known member
The synthetic steroid 7alpha-methyl-19-nortestosterone (MENT) is a potent androgen that is resistant to 5-reductase. It thus has decreased activity at the prostate and may have advantages over testosterone-based regimens in long term treatment or as part of a male contraceptive. Administration to eugonadal men results in suppression of gonadotropins, but its ability to support androgen-dependent behavior has not been investigated. For sustained release administration, MENT acetate was used, because its diffusion characteristics were more suitable for use in implants. However, upon release the acetate is rapidly hydrolyzed, and MENT is the biologically active moiety in circulation. We studied the effects of MENT on sexual interest and activity, spontaneous erection, and mood states in comparison with testosterone enanthate (TE) in 20 Caucasian and Chinese hypogonadal men recruited in Edinburgh and Hong Kong (n = 10 in each center). Outcomes were measured using a combination of daily diaries, semistructured interviews, and questionnaires. Nocturnal penile tumescence (NPT) was also recorded in the Edinburgh group. After withdrawal of androgen replacement treatment (wash-out phase) for a minimum of 6 weeks, subjects were randomized to two groups in a cross-over design. Drug treatment regimens were of 6-week duration and consisted of two implants, each containing 115 mg MENT acetate, inserted sc into the upper arm and removed after 6 weeks and two injections of TE (200 mg, im) 3 weeks apart. MENT treatment resulted in stable plasma MENT concentrations of 1.4 ± 0.1 nmol/L after 3 weeks and 1.3 ± 0.1 nmol/L after 6 weeks (mean ± SEM; all men). Nadir testosterone concentrations were 3.6 ± 0.6 nmol/L at the end of the wash-out phase and 9.4 ± 0.6 nmol/L 3 weeks after each injection. There were no differences in hormone concentrations between centers. There were no adverse toxicological effects.
There were only minor differences between the two treatments. Both MENT and TE treatment resulted in significant increases in sexual interest and activity, spontaneous erection (both by self-report and NPT measurement), and increases in positive moods, with decreases in negative moods in the Edinburgh group. In the Hong Kong group, both treatments increased waking erection, with a trend toward increased sexual interest and activity. Mood states appeared to be less affected during the wash-out phase than in Edinburgh men and showed no significant response to either treatment. These results demonstrate that MENT has similar effects on sexual activity and mood states as testosterone in hypogonadal men. As NPT is a physiological androgen-dependant outcome, these data provide further evidence for the androgenicity of MENT. The lack of detected effect of either androgen in Hong Kong men other than on waking erection illustrates the importance of the cultural context of symptomatology and its measurement. The appropriate dose of MENT remains to be determined, but these results support its development as a potential androgen replacement therapy.
Introduction
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
ANDROGEN therapy is predominantly used for replacement in primary hypogonadism. Other potential indications include the treatment of secondary hypogonadism, particularly that associated with aging, and as a hormonal male contraceptive that will involve long term treatment of large numbers of healthy men (1). Recent developments in androgen administration (2, 3, 4, 5) have addressed some of the problems with previous preparations, but are all based on treatment with testosterone. The use of synthetic androgens allows the possibility of more specific effects tailored to particular indications. 7-Methyl-19-nortestosterone (MENT) is a synthetic androgen that is approximately 10 times more potent than testosterone in anabolic bioassays and as a suppressor of gonadotropin secretion, but it is resistant to 5-reduction. It therefore has relatively low potency in bioassays in which the testosterone-amplifying activity of 5-reductase is important, such as stimulation of prostate size in castrate animals (6, 7, 8). This may be an advantage in long term treatment (9). Human data on the androgenic effects of MENT are, however, currently limited to the demonstration of the ability of repeated injections to suppress gonadotropin secretion in normal men (10) and early studies of masculinizing effects in female breast cancer patients (11).
MENT is not bound by sex hormone-binding globulin and is cleared rapidly from the circulation (12). MENT acetate (MENT Ac) can, however, be prepared in the form of implants for subdermal insertion, thus giving the potential for long term replacement therapy or treatment. Although MENT has been demonstrated to restore sexual behavior in castrate male mice (13), there are no data on the ability of this steroid to provide androgen replacement in men. This study was therefore performed to assess the effects of MENT Ac-containing implants on sexual function and mood in hypogonadal men.
Subjects and Methods
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
Subjects and treatments
Two groups of 10 hypogonadal men were recruited in Edinburgh, Scotland (all Caucasian), and in Hong Kong (all ethnic Chinese). Diagnoses were idiopathic hypogonadotropic hypogonadism (11 men), Kleinfelter’s syndrome (5 men), bilateral orchidectomy (2 men), following treatment for pituitary tumor (3 men), and cytomegalic adrenal hypoplasia (1 man). The mean age was 31 yr (range, 20–42) in the Edinburgh group and 38 yr (range, 28–47) in the Hong Kong group; the mean body mass indexes were 26 and 27 kg/m2 in the 2 groups, respectively (range, 21–35 in both groups). All men in the Edinburgh group and 7 in the Hong Kong group had received androgen replacement before recruitment; 3 men in the Hong Kong group had never previously received androgen replacement. All men were in good general health apart from their primary diagnosis, and hematological and biochemical analyses were normal. After recruitment, subjects entered an androgen wash-out phase of a minimum of 6 weeks for those receiving injectable or transdermal replacement therapy. The 2 men (both in the Edinburgh group) who were routinely being treated with sc testosterone pellets had subphysiological testosterone concentrations (<7 nmol/L) and showed symptoms of hypogonadism before entry to the study; testosterone pellets had been administered to these men 26 and 29 weeks previously. Plasma testosterone concentrations were in all men determined to be in the hypogonadal range (all <9 nmol/L) before starting MENT or testosterone enanthate (TE) treatment. The study was a randomized cross-over design with 2 6-week treatment phases comparing MENT with TE. Thus, in each center 5 men received MENT followed by TE, and 5 men TE followed by MENT. MENT treatment consisted of 2 implants containing MENT acetate inserted sc in the upper arm and removed after 6 weeks. The implants were prepared by the Population Council (New York, NY); each was 4.4 cm long and contained 115 mg MENT acetate, releasing 350–400 µg/day in vitro (Sundaram, K., unpublished data). Implants were inserted sc under the medial surface of the upper arm using a metal trocar with sterile technique under local anesthesia. Removal was also carried out under local anesthesia. Incisions were closed with sterile tape, and a bandage was applied for 24 h. TE treatment consisted of 200 mg TE (Primoteston, Schering AG, Berlin, Germany), im, repeated after 3 weeks to give a 6-week duration of treatment.
All men gave written informed consent before study entry, and ethical approval was granted from the local committees in Edinburgh and Hong Kong. The study was conducted according to good clinical practice guidelines under an approved Investigational New Drug license by the FDA.
Behavioral assessment
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
Sexual activity, interest, and mood states were assessed by a combination of self-report in the form of daily diaries, semistructured interviews, and structured checklists.
Daily diaries (14) were completed throughout the study, starting a minimum of 3 weeks before the first androgen treatment. Information collected included erection on waking (full, partial, or none; scored 2, 1, and 0); occurrence of masturbation and sexual intercourse; overall level of the following moods during the day: cheerful, lethargic, depressed, energetic, or irritable; and interest in sex during the day. Moods and interest in sex were scored from 0 ("not at all") to 4 ("extremely"). To reduce the impact of treatment carryover, data were derived from the last 21 days of each treatment phase of the study. Diaries were translated into Chinese in Hong Kong.
Semistructured interviews, modified from the report by Cawood and Bancroft (15), were used to record the frequency of sexual intercourse and masturbation over the last 2 weeks of each phase of the study.
SES 2 is one scale of the Frenken Sexual Experience Scales (16). This measuring instrument has a substantial body of normative data and has been shown to have satisfactory psychometric properties. SES 2 (the psychosexual stimulation scale) refers to the extent that someone seeks or allows (rather than avoids or rejects) sexual stimuli of an auditory-visual or imaginary kind. Weighted scores were used, giving a mean score of zero for normative data as obtained by Frenken and Vennix. We have previously demonstrated a small increase in SES 2 when eugonadal men were treated with supraphysiological doses of testosterone (14); thus, it appears to be a useful measure of androgenic response in men. To avoid confusion we have reversed the sign of the score so that a high score means high allowance of sexual stimuli or higher psychosexual arousability. As the questions in this scale refer to noninteractional social-sexual situations, it is appropriate for a study population who do not all have current sexual partners.
Nocturnal penile tumescence (NPT)
NPT was measured in the Edinburgh group on 2 consecutive nights at the end of the androgen wash-out phase and at the end of each treatment phase. Recordings were carried out in the subject’s home using a Rigiscan device (Dacomed Corp., Bloomington, MN) as previously described (17). The first night was used to allow acclimatization, and the second night was used for data recording. Two subjects declined to use the device. The following measures were derived as defined by Carani et al. (18): total number of erections, number of satisfactory erections, maximum percent increase in circumference lasting more than 5 min, maximum percent rigidity lasting at least 5 min in any one erectile response, total time that the circumference increase was more than 30% from baseline, and total time that rigidity was more than 60%.
Blood sampling and hormone assays
Blood samples were obtained between 0730–1100 h. Serum testosterone was monitored during the wash-out phase to ensure that plasma concentrations were in the hypogonadal range before MENT or TE treatment. Further blood samples were obtained at the time of commencing the first treatment phase and at 3-week intervals thereafter. Samples were separated by centrifugation, and plasma was stored at -20 C until assay. Full blood count, clinical chemistry, and lipid analyses were determined by routine autoanalyzer.
Plasma concentrations of MENT and total testosterone were measured by RIA in the Steroid Research Laboratory, Institute of Biomedicine, University of Helsinki (Helsinki, Finland), as previously described (10, 19) for MENT and using WHO matched assay reagents for testosterone (20). All MENT measurements were derived from a single assay, and all testosterone measurements from each subject were also performed in a single assay. Because of cross-reactivity of testosterone (2%) and other serum factors in the MENT assay, the mean (±SEM) value obtained before MENT insertion (0.67 ± 0.03 nmol/L) was subtracted from values of samples taken during MENT administration. The intraassay coefficient of variation for MENT was 5% at testosterone concentrations below 5 nmol/L.
Statistical analyses
Average scores for waking erection, the five mood states, and interest in sex were calculated for the last 21 days of each phase of the study as recorded in the daily diaries. The frequencies of masturbation and sexual intercourse were calculated as proportion of days on which the sexual activity was recorded. As not all men had sexual partners, the data for masturbation and sexual intercourse were combined to give a measure of overall sexual activity, i.e. the proportion of days on which either sexual intercourse or masturbation took place. Overall sexual activity was similarly calculated from the semistructured interviews, although as these outcomes could have occurred more than once in a single day, they are not directly comparable with those obtained from the daily diaries.
The first stage of the analysis was to test for a difference between the two centers at Edinburgh and Hong Kong for all outcomes. As the distribution of the average scores was not always continuous and was often skewed, with a large proportion of men recording the same value, transformation to normality was not possible. Nonparametric tests (Mann-Whitney U test) were therefore used for all of the daily diary and semistructured interview outcomes. Population SES 2 scores have a normal distribution (16), and plots of our data did not suggest the contrary; parametric tests (two-sample t test) were therefore used.
Significant differences between the centers were found for some outcomes for the wash-out phase. Such differences will not invalidate the analysis due to the cross-over study design, as treatment comparisons are within subjects. However, if the change in behavior seen when comparing an outcome under treatment with that seen during androgen wash-out is related to the center, this would suggest that the data should not be combined and that analysis should be by center. As a significant center difference was found in several of the outcome measures, all analyses were performed separately for Edinburgh and Hong Kong.
For analysis of a cross-over study design, the period effect and interaction between period and treatment effect must first be found to be nonsignificant before testing for a treatment effect (all outcomes by Mann-Whitney U test, except SES 2 scores by two-sample t test). If this is not the case, e.g. if there are carryover effects, then a comparison should be made of only the first given treatment as two independent samples. It can be assumed that the period and interaction between period and treatment were not significant for any outcome if no mention is made of these tests in Results. Comparisons between the two treatments (MENT vs. TE) and between each treatment and the wash-out phase were tested by the Wilcoxon signed rank test for all outcomes, except SES 2, by paired t test.
Serum testosterone and MENT data were analyzed by t test.
Results
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
Subjects and adverse events
Insertion and removal of the MENT Ac implants was well tolerated and was performed without complication in all cases. There were no withdrawals after recruitment in either center. There were no cases of abnormalities of blood cell count, clinical chemistry, or lipids (total cholesterol, high density lipoprotein, low density lipoprotein, or triglycerides) in any individual.
MENT and testosterone concentrations
MENT Ac administration resulted in significant increases in MENT concentrations at both 3 and 6 weeks over wash-out phase assay blank values (P < 0.001). Serum MENT concentrations were similar 3 and 6 weeks after administration (Table 1), indicating stable diffusion from the implant. Serum testosterone concentrations were increased during TE treatment, with similar concentrations at 3 and 6 weeks, and during MENTAc administration they were similar to those during the wash-out phase. There were no significant differences in concentrations of either hormone between the Edinburgh and Hong Kong groups at any time point. It should be noted that the concentration of testosterone measured during TE treatment is the nadir, 3 weeks after administration of TE.
View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Table 1. Plasma concentrations of testosterone and MENT at each stage of the study
After removal, the amount of MENT acetate remaining in the implants was measured. From this, the average daily release rate was calculated to be 461 ± 55 µg/day·implant.
Sexual interest and activity
Daily diary scores for interest in sex increased during treatment with both androgens. However, there was evidence of a period effect in the Edinburgh group, i.e. there was a greater increase during the second treatment phase regardless of treatment order (P = 0.032). Further analysis was therefore confined to data from the first treatment phase. This demonstrated a significant increase in interest in sex with both MENT and TE in the Edinburgh group (both P = 0.043 vs. wash-out phase; Fig. 1a). There was also an increase during both treatments in the Hong Kong group, but this did not reach statistical significance.
View larger version (32K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 1. Effect of MENT and TE treatment on interest in sex as recorded in daily diaries (a) and SES2 score (b). Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Interest in sex is the daily score in the wash-out phase and the first treatment phase, as there was evidence of a period effect; thus, n = 10 for the wash-out phase, and n = 5/treatment. Data represent the median ± interquartile range. n = 10 for SES 2 score, which is given as the mean ± SD. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.02 [vs. wash-out phase, by Wilcoxon signed rank test (a) or paired t test (b)].
The SES 2 score increased during both treatments in the Edinburgh group (P = 0.010, MENT; P = 0.028, TE; Fig. 1b). SES 2 also increased during MENT treatment in Hong Kong (P = 0.033), but the increase during TE treatment in that center did not reach statistical significance. There were no significant differences between scores during MENT and TE treatment in either group, although the difference approached significance (P = 0.051) in the Hong Kong group.
Changes in sexual activity as recorded in daily diaries and structured interviews are shown in Fig. 2. In the Edinburgh group, the daily diaries showed a marked increase in frequency of masturbation (reaching statistical significance during TE treatment, P = 0.030) with a small increase in the frequency of sexual intercourse. There was a borderline significant increase in overall sexual activity in the Edinburgh group during both treatments (P = 0.050, MENT; P = 0.051, TE). There were no significant changes in the Hong Kong group. Data from structured interviews confirmed that the effect of treatment was more consistently seen in the Edinburgh group with significant increases in masturbation during MENT treatment (P = 0.017) and sexual intercourse with both androgens (P = 0.027, MENT; P = 0.034, TE). The frequency of masturbation was greater during MENT than TE treatment in the Edinburgh group (P = 0.041). Overall sexual activity was increased with both androgens (P = 0.012 for both treatments) in the Edinburgh group, whereas the increases in the Hong Kong group were not significant. One man in the Edinburgh group and two men in the Hong Kong group did not record any sexual activity in the daily diaries at any stage of the study, and only one man, in the Hong Kong group, did not record any in the interviews. Two and three men in the Edinburgh group and five and four men in the Hong Kong group recorded in the diaries no masturbation and no sexual intercourse, respectively, at any stage of the study. Corresponding numbers for the interviews were one and three men in the Edinburgh group and five and four men in the Hong Kong group. Figure 2 refers only to those men who did not have zero outcomes throughout the study, whereas the statistical analysis applies to both the full dataset of 20 men and to these subgroups of men, as the Wilcoxon test is not affected by unchanged scores.
View larger version (45K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 2. Effects of MENT and TE on sexual activity as derived from daily diaries (a) and structured interviews (b). For diary data, sexual activity was calculated as the proportion of days on which sexual activity (sexual intercourse and/or masturbation) occurred. For interview data, sexual activity was calculated as the average frequency per day. Data represent the median ± interquartile range. Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Diary data: Edinburgh group, n = 8, 7, and 9; Hong Kong group, n = 5, 6, and 8 for masturbation, sexual intercourse, and overall sexual activity, respectively. Interview data: Edinburgh group, n = 9, 7, and 10; Hong Kong group, n = 5, 6, and 9 for masturbation, sexual intercourse, and overall sexual activity, respectively. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.02 (vs. wash-out phase); , P < 0.05 (MENT vs. TE treatment; all comparisons by Wilcoxon signed rank test).
Waking erection and NPT
There was a significant increase in the waking erection score with both treatments (Fig. 3a). This increase was significant in both centers (P = 0.008 for both MENT and TE in both centers). There was a greater effect of MENT than TE in the Edinburgh group (P = 0.007, MENT vs. TE), but no difference between treatments in the Hong Kong group. Similar results were obtained when data were analyzed according to the proportion of days on which an erection was recorded.
View larger version (44K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 3. a, Effect of MENT and TE treatment on waking erection in the Edinburgh and Hong Kong groups; b—d, NPT data from the Edinburgh group. B, Total number of erectile responses and number of satisfactory erections; c, the total duration that there was an increase in circumference of more than 30% from baseline and the total duration that rigidity was more than 60% in minutes; d, the maximum percent increase in circumference and the percent maximum rigidity lasting at least 5 minin any one erectile response. Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Data represent the median ± interquartile range. n = 10 for waking erection in each group; n = 8 for NPT data. *, P < 0.05; ***, P < 0.01 (vs. wash-out phase); , P < 0.01 (MENT vs. TE treatment; all by Wilcoxon signed rank test).
NPT data was only obtained for the Edinburgh group. There were significant increases in the number of satisfactory erections, the total number of erections (TE only), the total time that the increase in circumference was more than 30% of baseline, the percent maximum rigidity lasting at least 5 min in any one erectile response, and the total time that rigidity was more than 60% (Fig. 3, b–d). There was no change in the maximum percent increase in circumference. There were no differences between the responses during MENT and TE treatment.
Mood states
There were significant differences between the Edinburgh and Hong Kong groups in three of the mood states at baseline, with the Edinburgh group scoring higher for depressed (P = 0.009) and irritable (P < 0.001) and lower for energetic (P = 0.005). Men in the Edinburgh group also scored more highly for lethargic and lower for cheerful, but these differences were not statistically significant. In the Edinburgh group, there were significant increases in the positive moods cheerful and energetic with both MENT and TE, and significant falls in the negative mood states depressed, lethargic, and irritable with both treatments (Fig. 4). There were no significant differences in any mood state between MENT and TE treatments. There were no significant changes in any mood state in the Hong Kong group with either treatment.
View larger version (37K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 4. Effect of MENT and TE treatment on mood states in Edinburgh (a) and Hong Kong (b) groups. Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Data are presented as medians and interquartile ranges. n = 10 in each group. *, P < 0.05; ***, P < 0.01 (vs. wash-out phase, by Wilcoxon signed rank test).
Discussion
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
The implants used in this study contained 115 mg MENT Ac, each releasing 460 µg MENT Ac/day, which is then rapidly hydrolyzed into MENT. Based on the relative potency of MENT and testosterone and the daily production rate of testosterone (7, 21), it was decided to use two implants in this preliminary study. More detailed pharmacokinetic data of MENT release from similar implants have been obtained in a study of normal men (22), demonstrating zero order kinetics over 4 weeks. In that study, two implants resulted in plasma MENT concentrations similar to those in the present study, and profound suppression of gonadotropin and testosterone secretion. The present results are consistent with zero order release, although the limited blood sampling precludes more detailed analysis. Subsequent measurement of the amount of MENT Ac still present in the implants after removal was consistent with the measured in vitro rate of release and suggests that they could provide replacement for up to 8 months. The dose of TE used was chosen to be slightly lower than that routinely used in clinical practice, and measurement of plasma testosterone 3 weeks after each injection confirmed that it was starting to fall into the hypogonadal range. This minimized carryover into the second treatment phase for those men receiving TE first in a dose previously demonstrated to improve mood state in hypogonadal men (23). Complete restoration of both sexual interest and moods occurs at testosterone concentrations below the laboratory normal range (23, 24, 25). Behavioral and physiological (NPT) analyses were confined to the second 3 weeks of each treatment phase to further minimize carryover effects.
Effects on sexuality
Although the overall effects of this study demonstrate convincingly that MENT is as effective as testosterone in reversing the effects of androgen withdrawal on aspects of sexuality, a number of differences between the two centers were observed. These were not confined to the effects of MENT and are of potential relevance to androgen effects in general. Given that the two centers in this study were situated in contrasting cultural contexts and that this is the first published study of such androgen effects involving a setting that is not in North America or Europe, these center differences are interesting and of potential importance.
Probably the most robust physiologically direct effect of androgens in the human male that is relevant to sexuality is the effect on spontaneous erections during sleep (NPT). A dose-response relationship between androgen replacement and waking erection (i.e. the last NPT of the night) has been demonstrated (25, 26), and androgen replacement increases the magnitude of NPT (24, 25, 27, 28). These effects are not secondary to changes in sleep patterns (25, 27). In this study a clear effect of both androgens on waking erection was observed in both centers, and this effect was further validated in the Edinburgh group by the predicted effect on NPT recorded during sleep. Therefore, in this direct physiological effect, no center differences were observed.
Sexual interest was measured in two ways, by a five-point daily rating and by the SES 2 questionnaire scale. Both measures showed a clear, significant effect in the Edinburgh group and a weak, mostly nonsignificant effect in the Hong Kong group. Similar center differences were found for overall sexual activity. In general, the effects of androgens on sexual activity have been less consistent across studies than the effects on sexual interest or NPT. However, the main type of sexual activity to increase with androgen replacement in the Edinburgh group was masturbation, reflecting the variable possibility for sexual activity with a partner in these hypogonadal men. Furthermore, it was in rates of masturbation during androgen replacement rather than partnered sexual activity that the Hong Kong men were most markedly different, with half the men in Hong Kong recording no masturbation at any stage of the study. This could well reflect a cultural difference in the acceptability of masturbation. The impact of the Ying Yang doctrine on Chinese culture, for example, encourages sexual abstinence as a way of achieving or maintaining higher levels of strength and vitality (29). This raises the issue of the cultural appropriateness of the measures used.
Effects on mood
In general, effects of androgen withdrawal and replacement on mood are less consistent than the effects on sexuality. Some studies have shown a beneficial effect of testosterone on mood in hypogonadal men (23, 25, 30, 31); others have not (26, 32). In the present study there was a clear center difference in this respect, mainly manifested as a difference in mood states during the androgen withdrawal phase. The Edinburgh men reported more depression, irritability, and lethargy and less energy and cheerfulness than the Hong Kong men during this phase. As a result, there was greater potential for improvement in mood states during androgen replacement in the Edinburgh group, and that was shown to be the case; the Hong Kong men did not change. It is questionable whether the effects of androgens on mood variables, with the possible exception of energy, are a direct physiological effect. They may depend more on psychological interpretation of the actual physiological effects that occur (e.g. how does one feel about being less sexually interested?). If that is the case, we would not only expect to find more variance in mood effects in general, but also the potential for cultural differences. For example, in a recent study of women from Edinburgh and Manila, Philippines, we found that the Edinburgh women reported more negative mood around menstruation than the Manila women, who were more likely to report certain physical symptoms, such as backache (33). Thus, we may not have been asking the right questions of the Hong Kong men in this study. Clearly, if androgens such as MENT are to be used widely and in different cultures, it is important that the apparent cultural differences identified in this report be further studied.
MENT can be converted by aromatase to an active estrogen, 7-methyl estradiol (34), but it is resistant to 5-reductase (6). The effects of MENT may thus in part reflect those functions of testosterone that are mediated by estrogen receptors, but to a lesser degree those functions that involve amplification by 5-reductase. The metabolism of testosterone is important in mediating its effects in several physiological systems, including aromatization in the control of gonadotropin secretion (35) and 5-reduction in the prostate (36). 5-Reductase is also present in the human brain (37); however, the isoform present in cerebral cortical tissue is type I rather than type II, which predominates in reproductive tract tissues (38, 39, 40). Although it is clear that testosterone is necessary for normal sexual function (41), the role of aromatization or 5-reduction in humans is uncertain (42, 43, 44). In castrate male rodents, MENT fully restores sexual behavior (45, 46), but does not restore aggressive behavior, in contrast to testosterone, which restored both (13). Effects on libido and potency were reported by only small numbers of men during administration of the 5-reductase inhibitor finasteride for treatment of benign prostatic hyperplasia (47); however, that drug has selectivity for the type II isoform not detected in human cerebral cortex (39, 40). The demonstration here of the effectiveness of MENT in restoring sexual function and mood in hypogonadal men provides indirect evidence that 5-reduction is not required for mediation of the influence of testosterone on these behaviors in men.
In conclusion, these results demonstrate that MENT is able to provide physiological and behavioral androgen replacement in hypogonadal men. MENT was demonstrated to have effects on two groups of hypogonadal men from different ethnic backgrounds, confirming the validity of the data. There were only minor differences between the responses to MENT and to conventional testosterone replacement therapy using a range of outcome measures, but the appropriate dose for this or other clinical indications remains to be established. MENT may therefore provide effective replacement therapy with a degree of selectivity at different androgen-responsive sites
There were only minor differences between the two treatments. Both MENT and TE treatment resulted in significant increases in sexual interest and activity, spontaneous erection (both by self-report and NPT measurement), and increases in positive moods, with decreases in negative moods in the Edinburgh group. In the Hong Kong group, both treatments increased waking erection, with a trend toward increased sexual interest and activity. Mood states appeared to be less affected during the wash-out phase than in Edinburgh men and showed no significant response to either treatment. These results demonstrate that MENT has similar effects on sexual activity and mood states as testosterone in hypogonadal men. As NPT is a physiological androgen-dependant outcome, these data provide further evidence for the androgenicity of MENT. The lack of detected effect of either androgen in Hong Kong men other than on waking erection illustrates the importance of the cultural context of symptomatology and its measurement. The appropriate dose of MENT remains to be determined, but these results support its development as a potential androgen replacement therapy.
Introduction
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
ANDROGEN therapy is predominantly used for replacement in primary hypogonadism. Other potential indications include the treatment of secondary hypogonadism, particularly that associated with aging, and as a hormonal male contraceptive that will involve long term treatment of large numbers of healthy men (1). Recent developments in androgen administration (2, 3, 4, 5) have addressed some of the problems with previous preparations, but are all based on treatment with testosterone. The use of synthetic androgens allows the possibility of more specific effects tailored to particular indications. 7-Methyl-19-nortestosterone (MENT) is a synthetic androgen that is approximately 10 times more potent than testosterone in anabolic bioassays and as a suppressor of gonadotropin secretion, but it is resistant to 5-reduction. It therefore has relatively low potency in bioassays in which the testosterone-amplifying activity of 5-reductase is important, such as stimulation of prostate size in castrate animals (6, 7, 8). This may be an advantage in long term treatment (9). Human data on the androgenic effects of MENT are, however, currently limited to the demonstration of the ability of repeated injections to suppress gonadotropin secretion in normal men (10) and early studies of masculinizing effects in female breast cancer patients (11).
MENT is not bound by sex hormone-binding globulin and is cleared rapidly from the circulation (12). MENT acetate (MENT Ac) can, however, be prepared in the form of implants for subdermal insertion, thus giving the potential for long term replacement therapy or treatment. Although MENT has been demonstrated to restore sexual behavior in castrate male mice (13), there are no data on the ability of this steroid to provide androgen replacement in men. This study was therefore performed to assess the effects of MENT Ac-containing implants on sexual function and mood in hypogonadal men.
Subjects and Methods
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
Subjects and treatments
Two groups of 10 hypogonadal men were recruited in Edinburgh, Scotland (all Caucasian), and in Hong Kong (all ethnic Chinese). Diagnoses were idiopathic hypogonadotropic hypogonadism (11 men), Kleinfelter’s syndrome (5 men), bilateral orchidectomy (2 men), following treatment for pituitary tumor (3 men), and cytomegalic adrenal hypoplasia (1 man). The mean age was 31 yr (range, 20–42) in the Edinburgh group and 38 yr (range, 28–47) in the Hong Kong group; the mean body mass indexes were 26 and 27 kg/m2 in the 2 groups, respectively (range, 21–35 in both groups). All men in the Edinburgh group and 7 in the Hong Kong group had received androgen replacement before recruitment; 3 men in the Hong Kong group had never previously received androgen replacement. All men were in good general health apart from their primary diagnosis, and hematological and biochemical analyses were normal. After recruitment, subjects entered an androgen wash-out phase of a minimum of 6 weeks for those receiving injectable or transdermal replacement therapy. The 2 men (both in the Edinburgh group) who were routinely being treated with sc testosterone pellets had subphysiological testosterone concentrations (<7 nmol/L) and showed symptoms of hypogonadism before entry to the study; testosterone pellets had been administered to these men 26 and 29 weeks previously. Plasma testosterone concentrations were in all men determined to be in the hypogonadal range (all <9 nmol/L) before starting MENT or testosterone enanthate (TE) treatment. The study was a randomized cross-over design with 2 6-week treatment phases comparing MENT with TE. Thus, in each center 5 men received MENT followed by TE, and 5 men TE followed by MENT. MENT treatment consisted of 2 implants containing MENT acetate inserted sc in the upper arm and removed after 6 weeks. The implants were prepared by the Population Council (New York, NY); each was 4.4 cm long and contained 115 mg MENT acetate, releasing 350–400 µg/day in vitro (Sundaram, K., unpublished data). Implants were inserted sc under the medial surface of the upper arm using a metal trocar with sterile technique under local anesthesia. Removal was also carried out under local anesthesia. Incisions were closed with sterile tape, and a bandage was applied for 24 h. TE treatment consisted of 200 mg TE (Primoteston, Schering AG, Berlin, Germany), im, repeated after 3 weeks to give a 6-week duration of treatment.
All men gave written informed consent before study entry, and ethical approval was granted from the local committees in Edinburgh and Hong Kong. The study was conducted according to good clinical practice guidelines under an approved Investigational New Drug license by the FDA.
Behavioral assessment
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
Sexual activity, interest, and mood states were assessed by a combination of self-report in the form of daily diaries, semistructured interviews, and structured checklists.
Daily diaries (14) were completed throughout the study, starting a minimum of 3 weeks before the first androgen treatment. Information collected included erection on waking (full, partial, or none; scored 2, 1, and 0); occurrence of masturbation and sexual intercourse; overall level of the following moods during the day: cheerful, lethargic, depressed, energetic, or irritable; and interest in sex during the day. Moods and interest in sex were scored from 0 ("not at all") to 4 ("extremely"). To reduce the impact of treatment carryover, data were derived from the last 21 days of each treatment phase of the study. Diaries were translated into Chinese in Hong Kong.
Semistructured interviews, modified from the report by Cawood and Bancroft (15), were used to record the frequency of sexual intercourse and masturbation over the last 2 weeks of each phase of the study.
SES 2 is one scale of the Frenken Sexual Experience Scales (16). This measuring instrument has a substantial body of normative data and has been shown to have satisfactory psychometric properties. SES 2 (the psychosexual stimulation scale) refers to the extent that someone seeks or allows (rather than avoids or rejects) sexual stimuli of an auditory-visual or imaginary kind. Weighted scores were used, giving a mean score of zero for normative data as obtained by Frenken and Vennix. We have previously demonstrated a small increase in SES 2 when eugonadal men were treated with supraphysiological doses of testosterone (14); thus, it appears to be a useful measure of androgenic response in men. To avoid confusion we have reversed the sign of the score so that a high score means high allowance of sexual stimuli or higher psychosexual arousability. As the questions in this scale refer to noninteractional social-sexual situations, it is appropriate for a study population who do not all have current sexual partners.
Nocturnal penile tumescence (NPT)
NPT was measured in the Edinburgh group on 2 consecutive nights at the end of the androgen wash-out phase and at the end of each treatment phase. Recordings were carried out in the subject’s home using a Rigiscan device (Dacomed Corp., Bloomington, MN) as previously described (17). The first night was used to allow acclimatization, and the second night was used for data recording. Two subjects declined to use the device. The following measures were derived as defined by Carani et al. (18): total number of erections, number of satisfactory erections, maximum percent increase in circumference lasting more than 5 min, maximum percent rigidity lasting at least 5 min in any one erectile response, total time that the circumference increase was more than 30% from baseline, and total time that rigidity was more than 60%.
Blood sampling and hormone assays
Blood samples were obtained between 0730–1100 h. Serum testosterone was monitored during the wash-out phase to ensure that plasma concentrations were in the hypogonadal range before MENT or TE treatment. Further blood samples were obtained at the time of commencing the first treatment phase and at 3-week intervals thereafter. Samples were separated by centrifugation, and plasma was stored at -20 C until assay. Full blood count, clinical chemistry, and lipid analyses were determined by routine autoanalyzer.
Plasma concentrations of MENT and total testosterone were measured by RIA in the Steroid Research Laboratory, Institute of Biomedicine, University of Helsinki (Helsinki, Finland), as previously described (10, 19) for MENT and using WHO matched assay reagents for testosterone (20). All MENT measurements were derived from a single assay, and all testosterone measurements from each subject were also performed in a single assay. Because of cross-reactivity of testosterone (2%) and other serum factors in the MENT assay, the mean (±SEM) value obtained before MENT insertion (0.67 ± 0.03 nmol/L) was subtracted from values of samples taken during MENT administration. The intraassay coefficient of variation for MENT was 5% at testosterone concentrations below 5 nmol/L.
Statistical analyses
Average scores for waking erection, the five mood states, and interest in sex were calculated for the last 21 days of each phase of the study as recorded in the daily diaries. The frequencies of masturbation and sexual intercourse were calculated as proportion of days on which the sexual activity was recorded. As not all men had sexual partners, the data for masturbation and sexual intercourse were combined to give a measure of overall sexual activity, i.e. the proportion of days on which either sexual intercourse or masturbation took place. Overall sexual activity was similarly calculated from the semistructured interviews, although as these outcomes could have occurred more than once in a single day, they are not directly comparable with those obtained from the daily diaries.
The first stage of the analysis was to test for a difference between the two centers at Edinburgh and Hong Kong for all outcomes. As the distribution of the average scores was not always continuous and was often skewed, with a large proportion of men recording the same value, transformation to normality was not possible. Nonparametric tests (Mann-Whitney U test) were therefore used for all of the daily diary and semistructured interview outcomes. Population SES 2 scores have a normal distribution (16), and plots of our data did not suggest the contrary; parametric tests (two-sample t test) were therefore used.
Significant differences between the centers were found for some outcomes for the wash-out phase. Such differences will not invalidate the analysis due to the cross-over study design, as treatment comparisons are within subjects. However, if the change in behavior seen when comparing an outcome under treatment with that seen during androgen wash-out is related to the center, this would suggest that the data should not be combined and that analysis should be by center. As a significant center difference was found in several of the outcome measures, all analyses were performed separately for Edinburgh and Hong Kong.
For analysis of a cross-over study design, the period effect and interaction between period and treatment effect must first be found to be nonsignificant before testing for a treatment effect (all outcomes by Mann-Whitney U test, except SES 2 scores by two-sample t test). If this is not the case, e.g. if there are carryover effects, then a comparison should be made of only the first given treatment as two independent samples. It can be assumed that the period and interaction between period and treatment were not significant for any outcome if no mention is made of these tests in Results. Comparisons between the two treatments (MENT vs. TE) and between each treatment and the wash-out phase were tested by the Wilcoxon signed rank test for all outcomes, except SES 2, by paired t test.
Serum testosterone and MENT data were analyzed by t test.
Results
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
Subjects and adverse events
Insertion and removal of the MENT Ac implants was well tolerated and was performed without complication in all cases. There were no withdrawals after recruitment in either center. There were no cases of abnormalities of blood cell count, clinical chemistry, or lipids (total cholesterol, high density lipoprotein, low density lipoprotein, or triglycerides) in any individual.
MENT and testosterone concentrations
MENT Ac administration resulted in significant increases in MENT concentrations at both 3 and 6 weeks over wash-out phase assay blank values (P < 0.001). Serum MENT concentrations were similar 3 and 6 weeks after administration (Table 1), indicating stable diffusion from the implant. Serum testosterone concentrations were increased during TE treatment, with similar concentrations at 3 and 6 weeks, and during MENTAc administration they were similar to those during the wash-out phase. There were no significant differences in concentrations of either hormone between the Edinburgh and Hong Kong groups at any time point. It should be noted that the concentration of testosterone measured during TE treatment is the nadir, 3 weeks after administration of TE.
View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Table 1. Plasma concentrations of testosterone and MENT at each stage of the study
After removal, the amount of MENT acetate remaining in the implants was measured. From this, the average daily release rate was calculated to be 461 ± 55 µg/day·implant.
Sexual interest and activity
Daily diary scores for interest in sex increased during treatment with both androgens. However, there was evidence of a period effect in the Edinburgh group, i.e. there was a greater increase during the second treatment phase regardless of treatment order (P = 0.032). Further analysis was therefore confined to data from the first treatment phase. This demonstrated a significant increase in interest in sex with both MENT and TE in the Edinburgh group (both P = 0.043 vs. wash-out phase; Fig. 1a). There was also an increase during both treatments in the Hong Kong group, but this did not reach statistical significance.
View larger version (32K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 1. Effect of MENT and TE treatment on interest in sex as recorded in daily diaries (a) and SES2 score (b). Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Interest in sex is the daily score in the wash-out phase and the first treatment phase, as there was evidence of a period effect; thus, n = 10 for the wash-out phase, and n = 5/treatment. Data represent the median ± interquartile range. n = 10 for SES 2 score, which is given as the mean ± SD. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.02 [vs. wash-out phase, by Wilcoxon signed rank test (a) or paired t test (b)].
The SES 2 score increased during both treatments in the Edinburgh group (P = 0.010, MENT; P = 0.028, TE; Fig. 1b). SES 2 also increased during MENT treatment in Hong Kong (P = 0.033), but the increase during TE treatment in that center did not reach statistical significance. There were no significant differences between scores during MENT and TE treatment in either group, although the difference approached significance (P = 0.051) in the Hong Kong group.
Changes in sexual activity as recorded in daily diaries and structured interviews are shown in Fig. 2. In the Edinburgh group, the daily diaries showed a marked increase in frequency of masturbation (reaching statistical significance during TE treatment, P = 0.030) with a small increase in the frequency of sexual intercourse. There was a borderline significant increase in overall sexual activity in the Edinburgh group during both treatments (P = 0.050, MENT; P = 0.051, TE). There were no significant changes in the Hong Kong group. Data from structured interviews confirmed that the effect of treatment was more consistently seen in the Edinburgh group with significant increases in masturbation during MENT treatment (P = 0.017) and sexual intercourse with both androgens (P = 0.027, MENT; P = 0.034, TE). The frequency of masturbation was greater during MENT than TE treatment in the Edinburgh group (P = 0.041). Overall sexual activity was increased with both androgens (P = 0.012 for both treatments) in the Edinburgh group, whereas the increases in the Hong Kong group were not significant. One man in the Edinburgh group and two men in the Hong Kong group did not record any sexual activity in the daily diaries at any stage of the study, and only one man, in the Hong Kong group, did not record any in the interviews. Two and three men in the Edinburgh group and five and four men in the Hong Kong group recorded in the diaries no masturbation and no sexual intercourse, respectively, at any stage of the study. Corresponding numbers for the interviews were one and three men in the Edinburgh group and five and four men in the Hong Kong group. Figure 2 refers only to those men who did not have zero outcomes throughout the study, whereas the statistical analysis applies to both the full dataset of 20 men and to these subgroups of men, as the Wilcoxon test is not affected by unchanged scores.
View larger version (45K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 2. Effects of MENT and TE on sexual activity as derived from daily diaries (a) and structured interviews (b). For diary data, sexual activity was calculated as the proportion of days on which sexual activity (sexual intercourse and/or masturbation) occurred. For interview data, sexual activity was calculated as the average frequency per day. Data represent the median ± interquartile range. Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Diary data: Edinburgh group, n = 8, 7, and 9; Hong Kong group, n = 5, 6, and 8 for masturbation, sexual intercourse, and overall sexual activity, respectively. Interview data: Edinburgh group, n = 9, 7, and 10; Hong Kong group, n = 5, 6, and 9 for masturbation, sexual intercourse, and overall sexual activity, respectively. *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.02 (vs. wash-out phase); , P < 0.05 (MENT vs. TE treatment; all comparisons by Wilcoxon signed rank test).
Waking erection and NPT
There was a significant increase in the waking erection score with both treatments (Fig. 3a). This increase was significant in both centers (P = 0.008 for both MENT and TE in both centers). There was a greater effect of MENT than TE in the Edinburgh group (P = 0.007, MENT vs. TE), but no difference between treatments in the Hong Kong group. Similar results were obtained when data were analyzed according to the proportion of days on which an erection was recorded.
View larger version (44K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 3. a, Effect of MENT and TE treatment on waking erection in the Edinburgh and Hong Kong groups; b—d, NPT data from the Edinburgh group. B, Total number of erectile responses and number of satisfactory erections; c, the total duration that there was an increase in circumference of more than 30% from baseline and the total duration that rigidity was more than 60% in minutes; d, the maximum percent increase in circumference and the percent maximum rigidity lasting at least 5 minin any one erectile response. Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Data represent the median ± interquartile range. n = 10 for waking erection in each group; n = 8 for NPT data. *, P < 0.05; ***, P < 0.01 (vs. wash-out phase); , P < 0.01 (MENT vs. TE treatment; all by Wilcoxon signed rank test).
NPT data was only obtained for the Edinburgh group. There were significant increases in the number of satisfactory erections, the total number of erections (TE only), the total time that the increase in circumference was more than 30% of baseline, the percent maximum rigidity lasting at least 5 min in any one erectile response, and the total time that rigidity was more than 60% (Fig. 3, b–d). There was no change in the maximum percent increase in circumference. There were no differences between the responses during MENT and TE treatment.
Mood states
There were significant differences between the Edinburgh and Hong Kong groups in three of the mood states at baseline, with the Edinburgh group scoring higher for depressed (P = 0.009) and irritable (P < 0.001) and lower for energetic (P = 0.005). Men in the Edinburgh group also scored more highly for lethargic and lower for cheerful, but these differences were not statistically significant. In the Edinburgh group, there were significant increases in the positive moods cheerful and energetic with both MENT and TE, and significant falls in the negative mood states depressed, lethargic, and irritable with both treatments (Fig. 4). There were no significant differences in any mood state between MENT and TE treatments. There were no significant changes in any mood state in the Hong Kong group with either treatment.
View larger version (37K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
Figure 4. Effect of MENT and TE treatment on mood states in Edinburgh (a) and Hong Kong (b) groups. Open columns, Wash-out phase; hatched columns, during MENT treatment; stippled columns, during TE treatment. Data are presented as medians and interquartile ranges. n = 10 in each group. *, P < 0.05; ***, P < 0.01 (vs. wash-out phase, by Wilcoxon signed rank test).
Discussion
Top
Abstract
Introduction
Subjects and Methods
Behavioral assessment
Results
Discussion
References
The implants used in this study contained 115 mg MENT Ac, each releasing 460 µg MENT Ac/day, which is then rapidly hydrolyzed into MENT. Based on the relative potency of MENT and testosterone and the daily production rate of testosterone (7, 21), it was decided to use two implants in this preliminary study. More detailed pharmacokinetic data of MENT release from similar implants have been obtained in a study of normal men (22), demonstrating zero order kinetics over 4 weeks. In that study, two implants resulted in plasma MENT concentrations similar to those in the present study, and profound suppression of gonadotropin and testosterone secretion. The present results are consistent with zero order release, although the limited blood sampling precludes more detailed analysis. Subsequent measurement of the amount of MENT Ac still present in the implants after removal was consistent with the measured in vitro rate of release and suggests that they could provide replacement for up to 8 months. The dose of TE used was chosen to be slightly lower than that routinely used in clinical practice, and measurement of plasma testosterone 3 weeks after each injection confirmed that it was starting to fall into the hypogonadal range. This minimized carryover into the second treatment phase for those men receiving TE first in a dose previously demonstrated to improve mood state in hypogonadal men (23). Complete restoration of both sexual interest and moods occurs at testosterone concentrations below the laboratory normal range (23, 24, 25). Behavioral and physiological (NPT) analyses were confined to the second 3 weeks of each treatment phase to further minimize carryover effects.
Effects on sexuality
Although the overall effects of this study demonstrate convincingly that MENT is as effective as testosterone in reversing the effects of androgen withdrawal on aspects of sexuality, a number of differences between the two centers were observed. These were not confined to the effects of MENT and are of potential relevance to androgen effects in general. Given that the two centers in this study were situated in contrasting cultural contexts and that this is the first published study of such androgen effects involving a setting that is not in North America or Europe, these center differences are interesting and of potential importance.
Probably the most robust physiologically direct effect of androgens in the human male that is relevant to sexuality is the effect on spontaneous erections during sleep (NPT). A dose-response relationship between androgen replacement and waking erection (i.e. the last NPT of the night) has been demonstrated (25, 26), and androgen replacement increases the magnitude of NPT (24, 25, 27, 28). These effects are not secondary to changes in sleep patterns (25, 27). In this study a clear effect of both androgens on waking erection was observed in both centers, and this effect was further validated in the Edinburgh group by the predicted effect on NPT recorded during sleep. Therefore, in this direct physiological effect, no center differences were observed.
Sexual interest was measured in two ways, by a five-point daily rating and by the SES 2 questionnaire scale. Both measures showed a clear, significant effect in the Edinburgh group and a weak, mostly nonsignificant effect in the Hong Kong group. Similar center differences were found for overall sexual activity. In general, the effects of androgens on sexual activity have been less consistent across studies than the effects on sexual interest or NPT. However, the main type of sexual activity to increase with androgen replacement in the Edinburgh group was masturbation, reflecting the variable possibility for sexual activity with a partner in these hypogonadal men. Furthermore, it was in rates of masturbation during androgen replacement rather than partnered sexual activity that the Hong Kong men were most markedly different, with half the men in Hong Kong recording no masturbation at any stage of the study. This could well reflect a cultural difference in the acceptability of masturbation. The impact of the Ying Yang doctrine on Chinese culture, for example, encourages sexual abstinence as a way of achieving or maintaining higher levels of strength and vitality (29). This raises the issue of the cultural appropriateness of the measures used.
Effects on mood
In general, effects of androgen withdrawal and replacement on mood are less consistent than the effects on sexuality. Some studies have shown a beneficial effect of testosterone on mood in hypogonadal men (23, 25, 30, 31); others have not (26, 32). In the present study there was a clear center difference in this respect, mainly manifested as a difference in mood states during the androgen withdrawal phase. The Edinburgh men reported more depression, irritability, and lethargy and less energy and cheerfulness than the Hong Kong men during this phase. As a result, there was greater potential for improvement in mood states during androgen replacement in the Edinburgh group, and that was shown to be the case; the Hong Kong men did not change. It is questionable whether the effects of androgens on mood variables, with the possible exception of energy, are a direct physiological effect. They may depend more on psychological interpretation of the actual physiological effects that occur (e.g. how does one feel about being less sexually interested?). If that is the case, we would not only expect to find more variance in mood effects in general, but also the potential for cultural differences. For example, in a recent study of women from Edinburgh and Manila, Philippines, we found that the Edinburgh women reported more negative mood around menstruation than the Manila women, who were more likely to report certain physical symptoms, such as backache (33). Thus, we may not have been asking the right questions of the Hong Kong men in this study. Clearly, if androgens such as MENT are to be used widely and in different cultures, it is important that the apparent cultural differences identified in this report be further studied.
MENT can be converted by aromatase to an active estrogen, 7-methyl estradiol (34), but it is resistant to 5-reductase (6). The effects of MENT may thus in part reflect those functions of testosterone that are mediated by estrogen receptors, but to a lesser degree those functions that involve amplification by 5-reductase. The metabolism of testosterone is important in mediating its effects in several physiological systems, including aromatization in the control of gonadotropin secretion (35) and 5-reduction in the prostate (36). 5-Reductase is also present in the human brain (37); however, the isoform present in cerebral cortical tissue is type I rather than type II, which predominates in reproductive tract tissues (38, 39, 40). Although it is clear that testosterone is necessary for normal sexual function (41), the role of aromatization or 5-reduction in humans is uncertain (42, 43, 44). In castrate male rodents, MENT fully restores sexual behavior (45, 46), but does not restore aggressive behavior, in contrast to testosterone, which restored both (13). Effects on libido and potency were reported by only small numbers of men during administration of the 5-reductase inhibitor finasteride for treatment of benign prostatic hyperplasia (47); however, that drug has selectivity for the type II isoform not detected in human cerebral cortex (39, 40). The demonstration here of the effectiveness of MENT in restoring sexual function and mood in hypogonadal men provides indirect evidence that 5-reduction is not required for mediation of the influence of testosterone on these behaviors in men.
In conclusion, these results demonstrate that MENT is able to provide physiological and behavioral androgen replacement in hypogonadal men. MENT was demonstrated to have effects on two groups of hypogonadal men from different ethnic backgrounds, confirming the validity of the data. There were only minor differences between the responses to MENT and to conventional testosterone replacement therapy using a range of outcome measures, but the appropriate dose for this or other clinical indications remains to be established. MENT may therefore provide effective replacement therapy with a degree of selectivity at different androgen-responsive sites